NEW YORK (Reuters) – As the investigation continues into last month’s fatal shooting of Trayvon Martin, the unarmed black teenager killed by a neighborhood watch volunteer in Florida, psychology researchers can point to one aspect of the tragedy: how easy it is to “see” that someone is holding a gun when he is not.
In the latest research, scientists found that simply holding a gun, as George Zimmerman was when he confronted Martin in a gated community in Sanford, Florida, has an effect.
“The mere act of holding a gun makes it more likely that you will perceive an object as a gun,” said James Brockmole, associate professor of psychology at the University of Notre Dame and co-author of an upcoming paper on that phenomenon.
“Does that mean that if the neighborhood watch captain had not been armed he would have perceived the situation differently? It’s impossible to say that about any specific situation, but our research shows it is certainly a possibility.”
Martin was walking from a convenience store and had put up his hooded sweatshirt when Zimmerman, a 28-year-old white man, saw him. According to 911 calls the police released last week, Zimmerman followed the teen in his SUV.
“This guy looks like he’s up to no good or on drugs or something. It’s raining and he’s just walking around, looking about,” Zimmerman told the 911 operator. “He’s here now just looking at all the houses. . . . He’s coming to check me out. He’s got something in his hands.”
Martin’s lawyer said the 17-year-old was on his cellphone with his girlfriend as the incident unfolded, telling her that he was being followed. She encouraged him to run. The case will go before a grand jury beginning April 10.
Recognizing objects is not a simple matter of vision, said Brockmole. Instead, emotions, beliefs, and expectations can all affect the ability to accurately identify objects, numerous lab experiments have found.
Perhaps the most notorious real-world example of that was the 1999 killing Amidou Diallo in New York City: the 23-year-old Guinean immigrant was shot 41 times by police officers who perceived him as brandishing a gun. Standing in a dark doorway, Diallo was in fact showing them his wallet. In that case, said Brockmole, “racial stereotypes and beliefs about criminality may have caused the police to see a gun” that wasn’t there.
But something else was at play in the Diallo case: the police were holding guns. That caused Brockmole and a colleague, assistant professor of psychology Jessica Witt of Purdue University, to ask whether “the mere act of wielding firearms have biased the officers to misperceive Diallo’s actions,” as they put it in a paper accepted for publication in the Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.
PERCEPTUAL BIAS
Brockmole’s team ran five experiments with 220 participants. In each, the volunteers held either a foam ball or a firearm (a Wii gun – a type of gun used in videogames – or a disabled carbon-dioxide-powered BB pistol) while images flashed by on a monitor.
Each image, lasting less than a second, showed a person holding a gun or an innocuous object such as a shoe, soda can, or cell phone. The person in the image was either black or white, bare-headed or wearing a black ski mask. In one variation, the participant did not hold a gun, but one was conspicuously placed in the lab.
The mere presence of a gun nearby did not influence how likely people were to mistake a shoe or other innocent object for a gun. But holding a gun was, the scientists report in the upcoming paper.
“We got a substantial effect,” said Witt. “Holding a firearm makes you more likely to see innocuous objects as guns.”
That result fit with other studies showing that “people’s perception of their ability to act influences their perception of the world,” Witt said.
For instance, she has found that when someone holds a long stick, objects seem closer – apparently because people think their ability to reach the object means it is nearby. People with broader shoulders perceive doorways as narrower than smaller people do, strong hitters see a softball as larger than poor hitters do, skilled golf putters perceive the hole as larger than poor duffers do, and out-of-reach objects look closer when people can hit them with a laser pointer.
“The perception system and the motor system evolved together,” Witt said. “They share circuitry, so it makes sense that one would affect the other.”
Whether that effect – holding a gun making someone more likely to “see” a gun – played any role in Sanford is impossible to say. But the proliferation of right-to-carry and concealed-carry gun laws makes that mistake more likely, say scientists. It might not even be necessary to have the gun in one’s hand.
“It’s all about intention,” Witt said. “If you can feel the weapon on your hip and intend to use it, my prediction is that the perceptual bias would be just as great. Based on our other research, the anticipation of using an object is just as powerful an influence on perception.”
Added Brockmole, “They say that when you hold a hammer, everything looks like a nail. That doesn’t seem so harmless when you think about what happens when a person holds a gun.”
(Reporting by Sharon Begley; Editing by Michele Gershberg and Paul Simao)
Related posts:
Views: 0